LESSONS FOR METHODOLOGY IN COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE
FROM A COMBINED NEUROCOMPUTATIONAL MODELING-fMRI PROJECT

John Bickle,
Department of Philosophy and Neuroscience Graduate Program,
University of Cincinnati, P.O. Box 210374,
Cincinnati, OH 45221-0374.
Email: bicklejw@email.uc.edu

 

Abstract

 

            Two prominent methodologies in recent cognitive neuroscience are neurocomputational modeling (often coupled with computer simulation) and functional neural imaging. Unfortunately, these methodologies are typically employed in isolation from each other. Yet they can be combined to overcome some methodological shortcomings of each. I demonstrate this point empirically by describing some recent results from a joint neurocomputational—functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) project (Bickle et al. 2001). This project begins with published models and computer simulations of the cellular mechanisms of frontal circuits that compute sequences of saccades from an initial fixation point (Bickle et al. 2000; Bernstein et al . 2000). Those models were built up from details revealed by anatomical and single-cell physiological studies of primate frontal eye fields (FEFs), various frontal “working memory” regions, and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). Activity by a computer simulation of the full model reflects realistic primate saccade sequencing.

            However, in order to get realistic performance from our simulation, we had to choose some architectural and computational parameters that cannot at present be justified neurobiologically. This is typical of most neurocomputational work constrained by “low level” cell-physiological data. In keeping with our explicit neurocomputational focus, we treat these assumptions as predictions about neural processing to be tested by the appropriate laboratory methods. A number of these predictions from the saccade command model turn out to testable using fMRI.

            To do so, we developed a sequential saccade task (block design) of increasing complexity. Subjects executed four cycles of blocks of 2-, 3-, and 4-step saccades and a control motor task. All saccade targets on each run appeared and extinguished during the latency of the first saccade. Subjects were asked to saccade to the targets in the order of presentation. During the entire 8.5 minutes (510 seconds) of the four cycles, we took 340 fMRI data points (one full brain image in 24 slices every 1500 milliseconds). Activation data were acquired at 3T using BOLD (Blood Oxygen Level Dependent)-sensitized T2*-weighted gradient-echo EPI. (Technical details will be presented and explained during my talk.) A 3-D Modified Driven Equilibrium Fourier Transform whole brain scan was performed in an axial plane to provide high-resolution anatomical images for co-registration of activation maps. Image post-processing was performed using Cincinnati Childrens Hospital Image Processing Software (CCHIPS©) developed in the IDL® software environment. Cross-correlation between the BOLD signal intensity time course and the reference function was performed for each subject on a pixel-by-pixel basis; pixels above a statistically appropriate correlation threshold were overlaid on the co-registered anatomical image. In this manner we computed activation maps for each subject. These statistical parametric maps were then transformed into Talairach space for composite analysis.

            To identify frontal regions subserving sequential saccadic activity, we first identified regions that were significantly activated during saccade activity in any of the contrasts with the control motor task. Then we identified voxels in the grouped data where the amplitude of the BOLD-sensitized fMRI signal was correlated significantly (p < 0.01) with the number of saccade steps per trial (i.e., with increasing “saccade burden”). These strategies clearly identified FEFs, ACC, and two regions of frontal lobe previously associated with working memory functions in humans. This verified the areas from which single-cell data was gathered in constructing our neurocomputational model. We then plotted time courses for each of these regions in each subject, graphing level of BOLD signal intensity against fMRI data point. These results validated a computational assumption-cum-biological prediction of our model, namely that FEF activity increases montonically as saccade burden increases. However, this analysis also disconfirmed an important assumption of our model. We had assumed, based on the single-cell data, that FEFs only have the capacity to compute two-step saccade sequences, and had to rely on frontal working memory areas to store dimensions of earlier saccades when saccade sequences involved more than two steps. Yet we didn’t see significant activity in frontal “working memory” areas until the four-step task. This result forces us “back to the drawing board” to increase the biological plausibility of our neurocomputational model in light of this new data.

            Besides their intrinsic interest, results from our combined neurocompuational modeling—fMRI study have two broader methodological/philosophical implications. First, we now have a piece of actual scientific research that treats purely computational and architectural assumptions of a neurocomputational model as predictions and tests them using methodologies like fMRI. This project disables the common worry that neurocompuational models aren’t scientifically valid because they cannot be tested or falsified. But the advantages of a combined neurocomputa-tional—neuroimaging methodology run in the other direction as well. fMRI has yet to transcend a “neo-phrenology” status among some neuroscientists. Partly this attitude is fed by the “fishing expedition” strategy employed by a lot of fMRI research. Our combined methodology generates specific predictions about regions of interest for fMRI and greater focus for the design of experimental tasks and controls. And when combined with neurocomputational models based on known cell-physiological processes, fMRI becomes directly applicable to bridging the current gap between cognitive and cellular/molecular neuroscience.

            Second, the neurocomputational operation our model proposes as implemented in the frontal saccade command regions (vector subtraction) generalizes to other frontal regions. Anatomically, primate FEFs consist of “standard frontal cortex with distinct granular layer”: the same type that constitutes most of frontal cortex (Parent 1996). Hence most frontal regions have the cell types, distributions, and anatomical connections capable of implementing the same computational operation as FEFs to compute ordered sequences of neuronal activation patterns. Frontal regions subserve many higher cognitive processes (e.g., language production, planning, problem solving, complex motor commands); and virtually every higher cognitive (and con-scious) process is “sequential” in a fashion similar to saccade sequences (Bickle, Worley, and Bernstein 2000). By combining biologically plausible neurocomputational modeling with functional neuroimaging to investigate the cellular mechanisms of sequential saccade commands, we are at the same time postulating potential, biologically plausible cellular mechanisms for many types of sequential cognitive processes, implemented by cytoarchitecturally similar frontal regions of the primate brain.

 

References

            Bernstein, M., Stiehl, S., and Bickle, J. (2000). “The effects of motivation on the stream of consciousness: Generalizing from a neurocomputational model of cingulo-frontal circuits controlling saccadic eye movements.” In R. Ellis and N. Newton (eds.), The Caldron of Con-sciousness: Motivation, Affect, and Self-Organization. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 135-161.

            Bickle, J., Holland, S., Schmithorst, V., and Avison, M. (2001). “Cellular mechanisms of saccade sequencing revealed using a combined neurocompuational—fMRI approach.” Proceed-ings of the First World Congress on Neuroinformatics (Technical University Vienna), 571-588.

            Bickle. J., Worley, C., and Bernstein, M. (2000). “Vector subtraction implemented neurally: A neurocomputational model of some cognitive and conscious processes. Conscious-ness and Cognition 9: 117-144.

            Parent, A. (1996). Carpenter’s Human Neuroanatomy , 9th Ed. Baltimore: Williams and Williams.